Jennifer A.
Masterson
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of
self-reference on the misinformation effect. Participants initially were
presented a slide show in which half were asked to relate the images to themselves while the other half were asked to relate
the images to an unfamiliar person. After a short delay, participants were
given a nar
Metamemory
refers to people’s awareness, knowledge and control of their memory (Matlin,
2002). When people are presented with misleading information about precisely
learned or witnessed material, they are likely to mix up the original
information with the misleading information (Loftus, 1991; Wright & Loftus,
1998). This phenomenon is referred to as the misinformation effect. Over the
past quarter of a century, hundreds of studies have been conducted that demonst
It has been
found that people are especially susceptible to being influenced when there is
a delay between when initial information is encoded and when the misinformation
is given (Harvley, Patel, Tucker, & Thrasher, 1996). Loftus, Miller, and
Burns (1978) found that misinformation effects are largest when there is a
large delay between initial presentation of information and the misleading
information and when testing comes close in time after the misinformation is
given. In their 1978 study, Loftus and her co-authors showed participants a series
of slides. In the sequence, a car stopped at an intersection and proceeded to
turn and hit a pedestrian. Half of the participants saw a slide with a yield
sign while the other half saw a slide with a stop sign. Twenty minutes to a
week after the slides were presented, participants answered questions regarding
the details of the accident. The questions consisted of ones that were
consistent with the original slides, questions that were misleading and
questions that were neutral. Loftus and her co-authors found that people who
saw inconsistent information were much less accu
In
many post-event misinformation studies, participants are shown a sequence of
slides depicting scenes, are misled about various aspects of the original
slides, and are then given a forced choice recognition task that requires the
participant to choose between the original item and the misinformed item
(Loftus et al., 1978). Some studies use tests of recognition that involve
yes/no questions, and others use multiple choice
questions to assess scores (Loftus, 1998). However, no matter which form of
test is used, all of them can be used to not only assess scores that reflect
the misinformation effect, but also investigate how the information is encoded.
In a study
conducted by Harvley et al. (1996), it was predicted that familiar stimuli
would be a good way of encoding information thus reducing the misinformation
effect. However, they did not find significant results to support this
hypothesis, which could be due to familiar stimuli not being a strong enough
form of encoding. Therefore, because research has shown that self-reference is
a good way to encode information, it is conceivable that it would in turn
decrease the misinformation effect.
The
self-reference effect (SRE) seems to result because “the self is a
well-developed and often-used construct that promotes elabo
Some
studies have also suggested that self-referencing may aid in the learning and
retention of course material (Hartlep & Forsyth, 2000). For example, D. R.
Forsyth and Wibberly (1993) asked students to evaluate whether each adjective
presented orally in a list was self-descriptive. When later asked to recall the
lists, students recalled more of the self-referent adjectives. Therefore, it is
plausible that teachers should use this method for helping students to retain
material for exams.
The
final part of misinformation studies examine the
“quality of the resulting memories by measuring some aspect of these memories”
(Loftus, 1998). In order to further explore how SRE influences the misinformation
effect, the present study uses a scale of confidence to assess the participants level of confidence during the acquisition
period. Harvley et al. (1996) also used a scale of confidence and did so as an
entirety of all twenty questions asked during the recognition tasks, which
allowed for
As
Harvley et al. (1996) hypothesized with a familiar stimulus,
I also predict that the use of a self-reference will decrease the
misinformation effect. Participants who are merely told to imagine someone else
partaking in the situations presented will be more apt to being misled during a
subsequent test when given a nar
As for
predicted accuracy, I expect that on misleading questions, people who used the
self to encode information would be more accu
Participants were 56 undergraduate psychology students who volunteered to partake in the study.
For presentation of the initial material, 46 slides were used. These slides were picture images created by a 35-mm camera projected on a television screen.
The nar
For the
final test, a set of 20 questions was devised in the following manner. There
were 9 misled questions, 6 reinforced questions and 5 control questions. Each
question was asked in a different manner (see Table 1). The nine misled
questions referred to information that had been misled in the nar
Two interpolated tasks were devised to serve as
delay periods between: (1) The initial 46 slides and the nar
This experiment was conducted as a 3 X 2 design. The misinformation effect variable was within subjects; all of the participants received misled, reinforced and control questions. The second variable had to do with whether the participant was told to imagine himself or herself as being part of the situation portrayed in the slides or if the participant was told to imagine someone else as being part of the situation portrayed in the slides. This variable was between subjects.
Participants
were exposed to seven different phases: the instructional phase, slide
presentation, interpolated task, the nar
After
reading the instructions, participants viewed the set of 46 slides and were
allowed 3 s to view each slide. Both groups, the
groups who was told to use self-reference and the group who did not use
self-reference, viewed the same set of 46 slides. After completing this task,
participants were allotted 5 min to work on a word find.
The
fourth phase of the experiment was the nar
The
sixth phase of this experiment was the acquisition test question phase. Here,
participants were instructed to answer a series of 20 questions regarding the
slide show. Nine of the questions were misled, where the nar
After
completing the forced recognition task, participants entered into the seventh
and final phase of the experiment. Participants were asked to record a
confidence percentage that reflected how accu
The
SRE was not demonst
An
ANOVA was also used to calculate any differences between unfamiliar and
self-referent groups in terms of confidence percentages. For this between
subjects ANOVA I found there to be no significant difference, F(1, 54) = .745, p>.05. On the other hand, when a
repeated measures ANOVA was run to look at how well the participants performed
on the recognition test in each of the three types of questions, there was a
significant difference, F(1, 84) =
5.88, p<.05. This showed that
although participants who used self-reference did not expect to do better than
those who did not use self-reference; they were
accu
Although this study did not support past research regarding the self-reference effect (Forsyth et al., 1993; Loftus, 1998; Rogers et al., 1997), it did support the preponderance of my hypotheses. Participants in both groups scored higher on questions that were reinforced than on questions that were controlled. This, as argued previously, is a result of the reinforced information being viewed twice, while the control information had only been viewed once. Also, participants in the self-reference group did have the lowest percentage of correct answers when the questions were misled. However, participants in the unfamiliar stimulus group scored higher on questions that were misled than they did with the other two types of questions.
Past research shows that Self-Reference is suppose to increase one’s retention of material (Lord, 1980). Although my findings did not support this research, I do still feel that it is a strong encoding mechanism. It is possible that participants were not entirely sure on how to relate all of the images to themselves, thereby not increasing their retention of the material.
In order
to further assess participants’ accuracy of confidence,
it may be necessary to include a scale for each of the twenty questions. This
could show a difference between the type of question asked and how confident
participants were in their answers depending on whether they used
self-referencing or not.
References
Forsyth, D.
R., & Wibberly, K. H. (1993). The self-reference effect: Demonst
Hartlep, K. L., & Forsyth, G. A (2000).
The effect of self-reference on learning and retention.
Teaching of Psychology,
27, 269-271.
Harvley, P. R., Patel, B. R.,
Tucker, S. J., & Thrasher, L. W. (1996). Meamemory
and the effects of familiar stimuli on misinformation effect. Unpublished manuscript,
Klein, S. B., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1986). Elabo
Loftus, E.
F. (1991). When a lie becomes memories of truth: Memory
distortion after exposure to misinformation. American Psychological Society, 3, 121-126.
Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integ
Lord, C. G.
(1980). Schemas and images as memory aids: Two modes of processing social
information. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 38, 257-269.
Matlin, M.
W. (2002). Cognition: Fifth Edition. SUNY Geneseo, Earl McPeek.
Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977).
Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 677-688.
Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The
self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371-394.
Wright, S. B., & Loftus, E. F. (1998). How
misinformation alters memories. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 71, 155-164.
Table 1
Question Type Question
A.)
Cordless
B.)
Rotary
Reinforced Steve snacks on a?
A.)
Banana
B.)
Apple
Misled Steve plays with his pet ____ before bed.
A.)
Cat
Steve watched a little TV and decides to make a phone call to one of his friends. Before bed, Steve decides to have an apple as a snack. Steve plays with his ped dog “Danny-boy” and wishes him a goodnight (Steve actually has a cat).
(This excerpt and questions came from the Harvley et al. study, 1996)
Mean # of questions answered
correctly and Standard Deviation from the mean
Group
Misled .46 .20 .68 .18
Reinforced .89 .13 .61 .16
Table 3
Table
3
T-scores for each of the three
types of questions in terms of group (self vs. unfamiliar)
Misinformed by Self vs. Unfamiliar 4.310 p<.01
Reinforced by Self vs. Unfamiliar -7.016 p<.01