Sensation Seeking and Collegiate Vulnerability
To Internet Dependence
Michael Lavin
Kelly Marvin,
Amber McLarney,
Lori Scott
St. Bonaventure University
Michael
J. Lavin, Ph.D.
Professor
of Psychology
Psychology
Department
St.
Bonaventure University
St.
Bonaventure, New York 14778
(716)
375-2488
E-mail:
mlavin@sbu.edu
Fax:
(716) 375-7618
Abstract
Past research on Internet dependence has suggested
that those who are Internet dependent spend more time on-line, use chat rooms
more frequently, play MUDs more often, and web surf more than non-dependent
Internet users, but few studies have examined how personality characteristics
interact with Internet dependence. This
study investigates how Internet usage, preferences and attitudes toward the
Internet, and Internet behaviors are correlated with sensation seeking. It was predicted that participants who were
higher in Internet usage and Internet behaviors would be higher in sensation
seeking. Results indicated that
Internet dependents tended to spend more time on-line, use e-mail, surfed the
web, used chat rooms, used MUDs, and visited cybersex sites more often than
non-dependent Internet users. However,
dependents scored significantly lower on sensation seeking, thrill and
adventure seeking, and excitement seeking than non-dependent Internet users. It
was concluded that dependents interact with the internet using a motivation
scheme dissimilar to the physical thrill and excitement that typically characterize sensation seeking archetypes
Introduction
Thousands of teens enter the collegiate world every year
with a desire to learn and experience new aspects of life. In many cases, colleges are making this
desire more profound by way of the Internet.
For example, the University of Arizona, Texas A & M, and the
University of California at Berkeley are just a few universities that offer
classes via the Internet. This allows
for more convenient learning since students can connect from any phone line, in
many cases, straight from their dorm room.1-3
Colleges and universities not
only use the Internet to conduct classes but they also use it as a tool to
enhance education and the disbursement of
information to the fullest. Many Universities use the Internet as a
means of obtaining information on everything from student grades to the next
university-sporting event. Students can
arrange their own chat-rooms and E-mail lists as an avenue to conduct study
groups or post feedback on classmate's papers. In addition, Internet access has
become available in many dorm rooms, making certain universities and colleges
more attractive to prospective students who view the Internet as an essential
tool for learning.
The availability of computers, expectancy to use the
Internet, and the comfort of not actually having to physically be in class is
extremely appealing to many prospective students. Kandell4 offers a caveat emptor suggesting college age
students are at risk of dependency for three reasons: (1) psychological and
developmental changes that occur during this period in the students’ life;
(2) access; and (3) expectations for
the students to use the Internet.
Internet dependency is described as "a psychological dependence on
the Internet and is characterized by a) an increasing investment of resources
on Internet-related activities, b) unpleasant feelings when off-line, c) an
increasing tolerance to the effects of being on-line, and d) denial of the problematic
behaviors"4(p. 1).
Internet availability is viewed as a step toward the next
millenium however, colleges and universities are already seeing warning signs
concerning the effects of prolonged Internet use. For instance, at Alfred
University, officials found a relationship between excessive Internet use and a
more than doubled academic dismissal rate.5 To curb excessive use,
the University of Washington chose to limit the time individuals can spend
on-line.6 Counseling centers
at several campuses such as the University of Texas, University of Maryland,
and Marquette have felt the necessity to offer support groups for students
concerned about their time on the
Internet. 7
Although Kandell4 addresses the possibility of
Internet dependency, no empirical studies have been conducted to determine how or why students develop a dependence toward the Internet. Young8 collected anecdotal
reports from Internet users who reported addictive feelings associated with
their Internet usage. Users who
reported addiction feelings were found to spend more time on-line than those
who did not report feelings of addiction.8 Young8 also found that those who reported feelings of dependency suffered
with problems in other areas of their life, which were attributed to their
Internet usage. Individuals reported
family problems due to the lack of time spent with families, as well as work
problems due to excessive Internet usage.
Most importantly, 58 percent of
Internet dependents reported a decline in their academic work. They reported lowered levels of studying, a
decrease in their grades, and missed classes due to their use.
Studies
have also been conducted off-line to examine the consequences of excessive
Internet use.9-11
Morahan-Martin & Schumacher9 conducted a which assessed
Internet usage. The term "pathological Internet use" was defined as
excessive Internet use that resulted in problems at work or school, as well as
problems within relationships. In
addition, pathological Internet use also applied to those who suffered from
distress, an increased tolerance to their Internet use and withdrawal when they
attempted to quit using the Internet.
Ten percent participants reported suffering from four or more of these
symptoms. One reason for this might be
the fact that pathological users spent more time on-line than those did with
few or no symptoms.9
Furthermore, the relationship between pathological Internet use and
loneliness, was explored and revealed that pathological users were lonelier
than those who had either few symptoms or no symptoms at all.9
Scherer10 examined Internet use among college
students. Five hundred and thirty-one
college students filled out questionnaires concerning their Internet use. Scherer10 developed a criteria
level to determine Internet dependency by using the DSM-IV criteria for drug
addiction as a guide. Those who
exhibited three or more of the ten symptoms were classified as Internet
dependent. Eight percent of
participants were classified as dependents using this criteria.10
Findings of this study indicate that dependents used the Internet more for
personal or leisure use, such as chat, MUDs, and games when compared to non-dependents.10 Scherer10 also found that those
who were classified as dependents had lower success rates when they attempted
to cut down on, or quit, their Internet use. Fifty-three percent of dependent
users reported successful attempts to quit, compared to 83 percent of
non-dependents
Anderson11 surveyed eight separate colleges,
both public and private, and found 9.8 percent of participants to be dependent
on the Internet. Participants were
categorized as dependent if they experienced 3 or more of a possible seven
total dependency symptoms. Anderson11
found the greatest difference between Internet dependents and non-dependents
was the amount of time the spend on-line per day (224 mins. vs. 73 mins.) with
dependents being on-line longer.
Kubey,
Lavin & Barrows12 conducted the most recent study designed to
assess Internet dependency and its effects.
A survey was administered to 542 students which contained 43
multiple-choice questions assessing Internet use, study habits, academic
performance, as well as personal questions. Results of the Kubey et al.12
indicated that nine percent of the participants labeled themselves as
psychologically dependent to the Internet.
Psychological Internet dependence was measured by having participants
rate themselves on a 5-point Likert-type scale, asking participants if they
thought they had become psychologically dependent on the Internet.
Participants who were classified as dependents study more often agreed to statements about their use of the Internet.12 These questions included thoughts that they should curb their Internet use, being told that they use the Internet too much, lack of control over their Internet use and guilty feelings about their use. Dependents also agreed that if they had more friends at school, they would spend less time using the Internet. Kubey et al.12 also found that dependent users had been using the Internet for more years than non-dependents as well as a longer usage (in hours) of the Internet by dependents. These findings are consistent with those of Morahan-Martin & Schumacher.9 In addition, Kubey et al.12 found differences among dependents and non-dependents in academic areas. Those who reported Internet dependence also reported more problems with schoolwork and missing class time, which was contributed to their Internet use. This investigation was designed not only to replicate Kubey et al.'s12 study but also as a way to study what personality characteristics Internet dependents possess. One of these possible characteristics is sensation seeking. Zuckerman13 found computer and video game usage was positively correlated with sensation seeking. Young8 found that individuals who reported themselves as Internet dependent spent most of there on-line time in chat-rooms and on MUDS (Multiple User Dungeons), which is an on-line version of Dungeons and Dragons. The interaction found in chat-rooms and MUDS resemble that of computer and video games, which Zuckerman has found to be positively related to sensation seeking.13 However, Internet usage has not yet been explored, but it has been suggested that Internet dependents could possibly be sensation seekers. It was hypothesized that those who are categorized as Internet dependents will be high in sensation seeking compared to their non-dependent counter-parts.
Three hundred and forty-two participants (134 males and 208 females) at a small western New York university participated in the study. Participants took part in the investigation voluntarily.
Attitudes
and Behaviors Toward the Internet.
Participants’ Internet usage was assessed via a series of questions
concerning their Internet usage, preferences, attitudes toward the Internet,
and Internet behaviors. Attitudes and
behaviors were assessed by using a 6-point Likert scale (1=strongly disagree
and 6=strongly agree or 1=never and 6=always).
Internet dependency was assessed on the basis of a score of 4 or greater
on at least 4 questions.
Questions consisted of such
items as length of time using the Internet (years), weekly Internet use (hours,
increments of one) and weekly use of specific areas of the Internet including
Web-browsing, electronic mail, chat lines, news groups or listservs, Web site
construction, online shopping and multiple-user domains (MUD’s).
Sensation Seeking. Participants completed Zuckerman’s 13 (1978) Sensation
Seeking Scale (SSS). The SSS is a
40-item questionnaire that examines sensation seeking as a whole and on four
separate dimensions: (1) thrill and
adventure seeking; (2) experience seeking; (3) disinhibition; and (4) boredom
susceptibility. Respondents rate each
item by choosing between two statements. Depending on the statement chosen,
scores were obtained by summing the number of statements that corresponded to
each dimension and an overall sensation seeking score was obtained.
Participants were contacted
via a mass electronic mail message to all students at St. Bonaventure
University. Participants were asked to
voluntarily fill out an on-line survey for psychological research on the
Internet and were ensured confidentiality. The survey consisted of both
attitudes and behaviors toward the Internet and the Sensation Seeking Scale. 13
To ensure anonymity,
students gave their responses via the Internet. Once they had completed the survey, their responses were
electronically mailed to an address. An e-mail address was provided for
students who might wish to communicate with researchers for follow-up questions
or comments about the project.
Results
On the
basis of their scores on the Internet questionnaire, 43 participants were
defined as dependent Internet users and 299 as non-dependent Internet
users. Analyses were focused on
comparing dependent Internet users to non-dependent users. To assess dependent Internet users on-line
usage, t tests were conducted.
As shown in Table 1, dependent Internet users were found to spend more
time on-line, use e-mail, surfed the web, used chat rooms, used MUDs, and
visited cybersex sites more often than non-dependent Internet users.
In
addition, Internet dependent users were found to stay up later (M =
3.09, SD = 20.50) than non-dependent Internet users(M = 1.78, SD
= 1.04), t (339) = -7.47, p = .001. Also, dependent Internet users tended to be more tired (M
= 2.54, SD = 1.40) than non-dependent Internet users (M = 1.38, SD
= .711), t (339) = -8.57, p = .000.
T-tests conducted on the Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS)
yielded significant results. Dependent
Internet users scored significantly lower (M = 18.00, SD = 6.46)
on the overall score for sensation seeking than the non-dependent Internet
users (M = 20.70, SD = 5.58), t (339) = 2.91, p =
.004 (See Figure 1).
A break down of the SSS showed that dependent Internet
users approached significance on the Thrill and Adventure Seeking (TAS)
subscale. Results indicated that
dependents scored lower (M = 6.33, SD = 3.23) than non-dependent
Internet users (M = 7.11, SD = 2.49), t (339) = 1.88, p
= .065 (See Figure 2).
Finally, dependent Internet users scored significantly
lower (M = 4.07, SD = 2.15) on the Excitement Seeking (ES)
subscale than non-dependent Internet users (M = 5.25, SD = 2.01),
t (339) = 3.58, p = .000 (See Figure 3). No other results reached
significance.
Conclusion
Research
has suggested that those who are Internet dependent spend more time on-line,
used chat rooms more frequently, played MUDs more often, and web surfed than
non-dependent Internet users. 8-12
In this study, results from the Internet usage ratings suggest that when
an individual is Internet dependent, they spend more time on-line, frequently
used e-mail, surfed the web, used chat rooms, used MUDs, and visited Cybersex
sites more often than non-dependent Internet users. These results are consistent with previous research.
Overall,
the trends found suggest that college students are becoming more dependent on
the Internet. Much of this may be due
to increased availability of computers on campuses, more Internet assignments,
and class on-line discussions.
Students, especially incoming first-year students, may also use the
Internet as a social crutch in their new environment. They may depend on the Internet to maintain ties with family and
friends. If people use the Internet mainly for communication with others
through e-mail, distribution lists, and so on, they might do so to augment
their social contact with others, expanding their number of friends and
affording interaction that might not be otherwise possible.
A
" sensation seeker," as defined by Zuckerman 13 is one who
actively seeks out new and exciting ways to receive pleasure, whether it be in
the form of social activity or risky adventures. Rushes of adrenaline from mild
or extremely dangerous events are common occurrences for the sensation seeker.
Previous research by Scherer10 has demonstrated that those with
Internet dependent characteristics viewed themselves as more sociable and less
introverted or avoidant. It follows
from these observations that one would expect sensation seeking to be
positively correlated with Internet dependence. Contrary to these expectations,
it was found that dependent Internet users tended to score significantly lower
on sensations seeking scale. In fact,
those who are Internet dependent also scored significantly less on the thrill
and adventure seeking and excitement seeking subscales. There are a number of possible explanations
for these findings. Possibly dependent
Internet users tend to be very sociable in their Internet usage but not to the
point of sensation seeking. Their
sensation seeking differs from the traditional sense of the concept. Instead of sensation seeking through various
physical activities such, as sky-diving, dependent Internet users are more
non-physical in their sensation seeking.
Quite possibly, the scale used to assess sensation seeking touched more
on physical sensation seeking as opposed to non-physical sensation seeking. Additionally, because dependent Internet
users have been found to be lonelier than non-dependent users 9 dependent
Internet users might tend to have fewer social connections and thus do no seek
sensation from physical actions but rather non-physical actions to compensate
for their loneliness. A sensation seeking scale aimed at more non-physical
sensation seeking, rather than physical sensation seeking might demonstrate a
relationship between sensation seeking and Internet dependence.
Further
studies could include Internet usage and social networks. There are some researchers who claim that
individuals social network decreases with greater Internet access, however,
perhaps people’s social network actually increases due to the ability to
communicate with a variety of people almost anywhere in the world. Additionally, sensation seeking could be
further explored by using a different sensation seeking scale.
References
1.
Hance
(1998). University of Arizona: The
Wired Campus, Intel. [On-line].
Available:
http://www.intel.com/mobile/student/wired/arizona.htm
2.
Fenton
(1998). Texas A & M
University: The Wired Campus, Intel.
[On-line].
Available:
http://www.intel.com/mobile/student/wired/ut.htm
3.
Luong,
L.(1998). UC Berkley: The Wired Campus, Intel. [On-line].
Available:
http://www.intel.com/mobile/student/wired/berkley.htm
4.
Kandell,
J. (1998). Internet addiction on campus-The vulnerability of college
students. CyberPsychology and
Behavior, 1 46-59.
5.
Campus
ties to Internet use to increased dismissal rate. (1996, May 20). National On-
Campus Report, 24, 2.
6. Greene,
M. (1996, January 16). Addicted to the
'net. USA Today, pp. A1-A2.
7. Brenner,
V. (1996). An initial report on the on-line assessment of Internet
addiction:
The first 30 days of the
Internet usage survey. [On-line]. Available:
http://www.ccsnet.com/prep/pap/pap8b/638b012p.txt
8. Young, K.
(1996, August). Internet addiction: The
emergence of a new
clinical
disorder. Paper presented at the 104th
Annual Convention of the American
Psychological Association held in Toronto, Ontario.
9.
Morahan-Martin, J., & Schumacher, P. (1997) Incidence and correlates
of
pathological Internet use in college students. Paper presented at the 105th Annual
Convention of the American Psychological Association held in Chicago.
10.
Scherer,
K. (1997) College life on-line: Healthy and unhealthy Internet use. Journal
of College Student
Development, 38 655-665.
11.
Anderson,
K. (1998). Internet dependence among
college students: Should we be
concerned? Paper presented at the International
Internet Conference presented in San Diego.
12.
Kubey,
R., Lavin, M., & Barrows, J. (1998) Heavy Internet use and collegiate
academic performance. Submitted to the International Communication Association. San Diego, CA, 1999.
13.
Zuckerman
Address reprint
requests to:
Michael J. Lavin,
Ph.D.
Psychology
Department
St. Bonaventure
University
St. Bonaventure,
New York 14778
E-mail: mlavin@sbu.edu
Dependent Non-Dependent
Internet Usage M SD n M SD
n p
Time Spend On-Line |
13.00 |
11.79 |
43 |
|
6.71 |
7.08 |
299 |
.001 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
E-Mail |
4.93 |
6.38 |
43 |
|
2.62 |
2.50 |
299 |
.000 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Internet Surfing/Web Browsing |
3.58 |
3.32 |
43 |
|
2.46 |
2.85 |
299 |
.019 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Chat Rooms |
2.07 |
3.83 |
43 |
|
.816 |
2.45 |
299 |
.004 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
MUDs |
.465 |
2.313 |
43 |
|
.110 |
.732 |
299 |
.041 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Cybersex |
1.860 |
1.246 |
43 |
|
1.418 |
.842 |
299 |
.003 |