Levels of Impulsivity and Stress on Accuracy Scores and Metacognition
Amanda Buck
1)
quite an improvement
over 1st draft
2)
you still could shave
some of the introductions articles since they are irrelevant
3)
I made a few grammar and
script changes
4)
Look forward to figures
(will have to have for presentation)
Abstract
This study
assessed the difference between how individuals with two different types of
impulsivity react to stress and how their metacognitive abilities differ as
well as how they interact.. Accuracy scores of the arithmetic task in
the stressful condition and the non-stressful condition, and perceived scores
were recorded and analyzed. The results showed main effects for level of
impulsivity and stress level(describe) , and no
interaction. Other tests were implemented to evaluate the characteristic of
impulsiveness, and stress levels. The
present study demonst
Levels
of Impulsivity and Stress on Accuracy Scores and Metacognition
Impulsivity is
“the tendency to delibe
Impulsivity has
been primarily assessed in terms of extraversion. Impulsivity is the trait of extraversion that
directly affects the most basic memory and perceptual processes and the way in
which they are carried out (Dickman, 1990).
When arousal or stress is increased extraverts tend to show higher
levels of performance (Corr & Kumari, 1998). Extraverts also have faster response times
for motor behaviors which puts them at an advantage under pressure (Doucet &
Stelmack, 2000). Research has begun to
focus on the effects of stress on impulsivity and on the distinction between
two different types of impulsivity: functional and dysfunctional. +
Dysfunctional
impulsivity has been widely studied. Dysfunctional impulsivity is “[?…] a tendency to initiate behavior before a situation
has been analyzed adequately” (Caplan & Shecter, 1990). Functional impulsivity is defined as the
tendency to act with relatively little forethought when such a style is optimal
(Dickman, 1990). Previous research found
that some traits related to functional impulsivity are enthusiasm, activity,
and adventurousness (Dickman, 1990).
This finding is consistent with the fact that these people report benefiting
from their impulsiveness (Dickman, 1990).
These individuals are more productive; they are also risk takers. It
seems that when functional impulsives make a high number of errors it is offset
by the amount of information they contribute to the task (Dickman, 1990). Functionally impulsive individuals work more
quickly because they are intelligent (Dickman, 1990).
People who are dysfunctionally impulsive seem to work in the opposite way. They do not report beneficial outcomes attributed to their impulsivity. This is because they are characterized by disorderliness and tend to ignore information necessary to make accurate decisions (Dickman, 1990). There is evidence that shows functional and dysfunctional impulsives react differently when under stress and have differences in cost during accuracy tasks (Dickman, 1990).
A stressful
situation is a situation that is threatening or demanding in which necessary
resources are unavailable to cope with the situation (Cohen, Kamarck &
Mermelstein, 1983). The stressor this study
addresses is time pressure and its affect on accuracy of individual performance.
Stress actually motivates performance (Corr &
Kumari, 1998).for whom Other effects that occur under
time pressure are accele
Research(all research is previous) also suggests that individuals
low in impulsivity are sensitive to stress provoking stimuli, causing them to
change their route of cognitive processing from automatic processing to controlled
processing. In stressful situations, effort will increase, causing cognitive
performance to improve (Corr & Kumari, 1998). These findings imply that impulsive people
are greatly affected by stress. In
addition, dysfunctionally impulsive people tend to have difficulty processing
information; they will have more of a deficit in accuracy when put under
pressure. Surprisingly, stress can
create positive mood states on certain occasions if the stressor is viewed as a
challenge and not a threat (Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000). This would be characteristic of functional
impulsives. Because individuals are
enthusiastic and highly productive, they are more accu
Accuracy in
solving arithmetic tasks is measured in this study. Research
proposed that stress causes participants to narrow their focus of attention and
have a difficult time performing complex tasks (Zautra, Reich, Davis, Potter
& Nicolson., 2000). Research has
illust
This study probes into the metacognitive abilities in functional
and dysfunctional individuals. Research has
suggested that personality plays a role in the accuracy of metacognition
(Pallier, Wilkinson, Danthiir, Kleitman, Knezevic, Stankov, & Roberts,
2002). When subjects are faced with time
pressure they are less confident about the decisions they make (Zautra et al.,
2000). Time pressure affects confidence
+
After considering
this information, it is hypothesized that when completing a mathematical task
under stressful conditions, dysfunctionally impulsive individuals will complete
the task with less accuracy than those who are functional impulsives. In
addition, I am predicting an interaction such that functional
impulsives will be more accu
Method
Participants+
Twenty undergraduates (10 functional impulsives and 10 dysfunctional impulsives) between the ages of 18-23 participated in this study and received extra credit. Each participant was placed in the stressful condition and in the non-stressful condition. Participants rated their performance according to the percentage that they thought they got correct on the math tasks after answering math questions in both conditions.
Materials
In order to assess the personality characteristic of impulsivity, the Eyesenck Personality Inventory Extraversion Scale (EPI-I Scale) was administered. Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity was measured by using the Dickman Impulsivity Inventory; DII (Claes, Vertommen, & Braspenning, 2000). Stress was measured using the Perceived Stress Scale; PSS (Cohen et al., 1983). This scale is designed to measure the degree of stress one experiences in his or her life in specific situations. A minor change was made for the purpose of this study. Instead of using the phrase “in the last month” it was modified to “during this task”. This scale is an accurate measure of life-event impact measures (Cohen, et al., 1983).
Two arithmetic tasks were implemented in order to control for learning effects. Each consisted of 15 problems, randomly assigned from Grabetter and Wallnau. A short questionnaire was given to all participants. The questions were 1. “How many math courses have you taken at St. Bonaventure University?” and 2. What percentage of the items on the math tasks do you think you answered correctly?”
Procedure
The present study was completed over a 2 day period. On the first day participants were given impulsivity tests. The Eyensenck Personality Inventory and the Dickman Impulsivity Inventory were used to determine impulsivity and the levels they exhibited. The DII was then used to distinguish between functional impulsives and dysfunctional impulsives. On the second day, the 10 participants who were functional impulsives were put into a stressful time pressure situation and told to complete a 15 question arithmetic task in 6 minutes. They were told to finish the questions as quickly as possible and to do their best. Every 2 minutes the participants were reminded of how much time they had to finish. In addition, the same participants were put into a situation that was not stressful in which the task was not timed. Participants were given unlimited time to finish the arithmetic task and told to take their time and do their best. After participating in each condition, participants completed the short questionnaire to determine their metacognitive abilities and to investigate whether there is was a difference between metacognitive abilities under stressful versus non-stressful conditions. Dysfunctional impulsives also went through both of these conditions and were given the short questionnaire. The participates were given the PSS immediately after completing the questionnaire to ensure that participants in the time pressure conditions were experiencing stress. After the task was completed, the number of correct answers on the task was measured. This is the accuracy score.
Results
The results were analyzed using 3(I thought you had 2 2x2s) 2 (functional vs.
dysfunctional impulsivity) x 2 (stress vs. no stress) analyses of variance. It
is a within subject design with two dependent variables. The two dependent variables are metacognition,
or perceived scores, and real accuracy scores. Direct
me to Figure 1
Three separate analyses of variance were used to analyze the data. The first analysis of variance measured real accuracy scores. There was a main effect for type of impulsivity F (1, 18) = 38.926, p <.05 and there was also a main effect for stress level, or time pressure, F (1, 18) = 4.539, p < .05 and no interaction. The second measured perceived scores, again, there was a main effect for type of impulsivity F (1, 18) = 26.982 p < .05 and a main effect for stress level F (1, 18) = 5.154 p < .05. Similar to the first, there was no interaction. In the third analysis of variance individual perceived scores were subtracted from real accuracy scores in order to determine metacognition, or how close they came to the real score when making predictions. These subtracted results were put into an analysis of variance and there was a main effect for type of impulsivity F (1, 18) = 16.012 p < .05 and a main effect for stress level F (1, 18) = 7.175) p < .05. There was no interaction for this analysis of variance.
Save for discussion In terms of metacognition, dysfunctional
impulsives were less accu
Under stressful conditions individuals were less confident in their judgments. Individuals who were more intelligent and had a history of doing well on tests, (functional impulsives) were under-confident in self-assessment and those who were less intelligent and did not do well on tests (dysfunctional impulsives) were overconfident in self-assessment (Palier et al., 2002).
Though the functional impulsives have high speed for information-processing and showed some inaccurate answers, their method of processing was found to be “functional” under the specific conditions of the experiment. Functional impulsives had higher scores and higher accuracy when answering the questions.(Dickman, 1990). Individuals who were dysfunctionally impulsive had lower accuracy scores.
Discussion
The results showed that functional impulsives did in fact have higher accuracy scores than dysfunctional impulsives. There was also a difference in stress. When in a stressful situation accuracy scores were lower than when the participants were placed in a non-stressful situation. The same results were found using perceived scores and metacognitive scores. The hypotheses predicted main effects. However, the hypothesis also predicted interactions, and this was not found.
There are some reasons the results did not completely coincide with the hypotheses made. For instance, the stressful condition may not have been stressful enough. The individuals may have been borderline dysfunctional or borderline functional. The extremes of this personality trait may have been necessary in order to result in a significant interaction. Some individuals may have been better in math than others and taken more math courses recently this could also pose a bias in the results.
(what happen to your font here?)In reference to
metacognition, individuals generalize their cognitive abilities, causing them
to predict consistent levels of confidence. Because of this pattern, confidence
In
future research it is suggested that participants be placed in a more stressful
condition, and extreme personalities are assessed. Metacognition could also be measured in
different ways that would be more effective.
For instance, one might look at how confident one is in his or her
predictions, rather than only the perceived scores. A different task could also be used besides a
math task to ensure that the situation was stressful. A different task could be used so that
learning bias does not take place. For
instance anagrams could be used. These
are all things that future researchers should delve deeper into. If this is done properly it may be possible
to find significant interactions.
References
Becker, P. & Schneider, E. (1976). Specific Reactions to Stress and Personality
Characteristics: Students Before an Examination. Aeitschrift fuer Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 23(1), 1-29.
Brunas-Wagstaff, J., Bergquist, A. & Wagstaff, G. F. (1994). Cognitive Correlates of
Functional and Dysfunctional Impulsivity in Adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 17(2), 289-292.
Caplan, B. & Shechter, J. (1990). Clinical Applications of the Matching Familiar Figures
Test: Impulsivity vs. Unilateral Neglect. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 60-67.
Claes, L., Vertommen, H. & Braspenning, N. (2000). Factor Analysis and Description
of Psychometric Properties of Dickman Impulsivity Inventory in Dutch Speaking Adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 29(1), 27-35.
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T. & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A Global Measure of Perceived
Stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385-396.
Corr, P. & Kumari, V. (1998). Impulsivity, Time of Day, and Stress: Effects on
Intelligence Test Performance. Journal of Research in Personality, 32, 1-12.
Dickman, S. (1990). Functional and Dysfunctional Impulsivity: Personality and
Cognitive Correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 95-102.
Doucet, C. & Stelmack, R. M. (2000). An Event-Related Potential Analysis of
Extraversion and Individual Differences in Cognitive Processing Speed and Response Execution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 956-964.
Folkman, S. & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Stress, Positive Emotion, and Coping.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 115-118.
Hacker, D. J., Bol, L., Horgan,
Performance in a Classroon Context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92 (1),
160-170.
Pallier, G., Wilkinson, R., Danthir, V., Kleitman, S., Knezevic, G., Stankov, L. &
Roberts, R. D. (2002). The Role of Individual Differences in the Accuracy of Confidence Judgements. The Journal of General Psychology, 129 (3), 257-300.
Verplanken, B. (1993). Need for Cognition and External Information Search:
Responses to Time Pressure during Decision-Making. Journal of Research in Personality, 27, 238-252.
Zautra, A. J., Reich, J. W., Davis, M. C., Potter, P. T. & Nicolson, N .A.
(2000). The Role of Stressful Events in the Relationship Between Positive and Negative Affects: Evidence From Field and Experimental Studies. Journal of Personality, 68, 927-950.
Zuckerman, M. & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and Risking Taking:
Common Biosocial Factors. Journal of Personality, 68, 999-1016.
Tables are not in yet Figures