Loneliness, Social Desirability and the Effect on Negative Metaperceptions
Teressa Davis
St. Bonaventure University
Abstract
Metaperceptions ( Kisler, 1998) are what one thinks others think of themselves;
there are
both negative and positive metaperceptions. My study is interested to
see if certain
personalities are more likely to have negative metaperceptions. Powling
& Hopes, 1988
noted that “the lonely tend to evaluate themselves and others negatively”
a characteristic
of negative metaperceivers but my investigation wants to ask whether
social desirability
interacts with loneliness to predict negative metaperception. Participants scoring high
in loneliness were predicted to have greater negative metaperceptions
than those scoring
low in loneliness; additionally those scoring low on social
desirability would have greater
negative metaperceptions than high social desirables. An interaction was predicted such
metaperception depended on both loneliness and social
desirability. Participants high in
loneliness and social desirability lonely and low social
desirable should be have the
greatest negative metaperception.The result showed that there was a main effect for
loneliness with high lonely have greater negative metaperceptions than
low. No main was
found for social desirability.An interaction was found between
loneliness and social
desirability as predicated. High lonely and low social desirable were
highest in negative
metaperception.
Loneliness, Social Desirability and the Effect on
Metaperceptions
Social desirability is the desire to be viewed in a positive light by others. Previous research (Porrata, 1993) has found that subjects who score high on social desirability tend to be more conforming whereas subjects who score lower are found to express themselves more freely.
Lonely people may feel as though they have no sense of belonging. Evidence in previous literature (Powling & Hopes, 1988) shows that “the lonely tend to evaluate themselves and others negatively, but that others in general do not view the lonely unfavorably”.
Metaperceptions
are what one thinks others think of them (Kisler, 1998). Albright,
If a
person is lonely, it is more likely that they will have negative metaperceptions. It is possible that one of the reasons a
person may be lonely is if they do not have many friends. A person without many friends could think
that other people do not like them.
Therefore, the negative metaperceptions could lead to loneliness and the
loneliness could also lead to the negative metaperceptions. Social desirability could also be attributed
because a lonely person may be lonely because of the low desire to please
others, also factoring in a reason why a person may have negative
metaperceptions. By researching this,
metaperceptions may be understood more and this research may serve as a tool to
work on negativity in the human race, considering that humans are very social
creatures and no one desires to be lonely.
Research (Leak & Parsons, 2001) has also shown that “participants have a tendency to present themselves in a positive light, and this can affect the validity of their self-reports.” There are two different types of desirable responding; the first is conscious response distortion, also known as impression management, and the second is unconscious self-deception, also known as self-deception enhancement. The first is a deliberate deception, while the second is an honest, although possibly inaccurate, overly positive self-description. Participants in the Leak & Parson’s study took a series of questionnaires to evaluate attachment styles and social desirability. This study is important to look at because the social desirability scale employed was a larger scale of the inventory to be used in the present study. This investigation found that the evaluations regarding the three attachment styles reflected an influence of the conscious response distortion. Evaluations concerning two of the three attachment styles were also found to be influenced by the unconscious self-deception. In other words, the results of the questionnaires were found to be inaccurate, due to the participants’ desire to appear favorably, whether it be conscious or unconscious. Several studies have been done regarding the relationship between social desirability and loneliness.
Loneliness can be described as not feeling as though one belongs. Previous research (Powling & Hopes, 1988) was done in which college students were evaluated for “desirability and acquaintanceship by members of other groups”. Results (Powling & Hopes, 1988) found that being less attractive and loneliness are significantly correlated. Lonely people may be less inclined to care about their appearance because of low social desirability. They may not care how others view them, therefore not paying much attention to their physical appearance.
Wittenberg & Reis (1986) studied the interdependence of social skills, perceptions of others, and sex-role orientation in pairs of first year college roommates. Results (Wittenberg & Reis, 1986) show that the “lonelier the subjects were, the more deficient they were in relationship formation and maintenance skills”. These subjects also had more negative perceptions of their roommates. The inadequacy of social skills and negativity were both found to be connected to loneliness together and separately (Wittenberg & Reis, 1986). Social assertion, acceptance and responsiveness to others were found to be important character traits in avoiding loneliness (Wittenberg & Reis, 1986). The results of this study support the notion that people who are not lonely may also have higher social desirability versus people who are lonely and tend to be deficient in their social skills and negative towards others.
Metaperceptions
are what one thinks others think of them.
Previous research (Albright et al.) focused on integrating self-presentation
and metaperception. Participants were
paired up and were assigned roles to act out.
The actors’ metaperceptions and their partners’ trait judgements were
measured. There was a “high level of
accuracy in actors’ metaperceptions but no accuracy in partners’ trait
judgements of the actors”. These results
are not suprising because the actors were trying to portray a certain
image. Their metaperceptions were
accurate which means they were successful in enacting the certain roles. In a study (Kisler, 1998) that compared
perceptions and metaperceptions among bulimic patients and another female
partner, the bulimic patients “thought that they had made a more negative
impression than they actually had”. There were no differences in how the two
groups of women actually perceived each other (Kisler, 1998). This supported
the hypothesis that the bulimic women would have more negative metaperceptions,
but did not support that they would also be perceived more negatively by their
female partners (Kisler, 1998). There was a significant amount of variance
among the bulimic patients and their female counterparts in social desirability
and their perceptions and metaperceptions (Kisler, 1998). The bulimic patients scored lower on social
desirability but scored higher on loneliness (Kisler, 1998). This helps to support the present hypothesis
that there will be a negative relationship between loneliness and social
desirability with an increase in negative metaperceptions. Kalliopuska, 1992)
has found that there is a significant positive relationship between the
willingness to help and high social desirability.
It is hypothesized that participants who score high in loneliness will score lower on social desirability and will have an increase in negative metaperceptions. It is also hypothesized that there will be a main effect for loneliness and a main effect for social desirability with an interaction of the two. There will be a higher score of people with negative metaperceptions if they score high in loneliness and low in social desirability. Positive or negative metaperceptions will depend on a participants score in loneliness and social desirability. Lonely people may have less of a desire to please others; therefore they may believe that others have negative perceptions of them. People who are high in social desirability tend to do things that they believe will please other people. The willingness to help can be viewed as an act that is done in order to please others.
Method
Participants
40 pairs of undergraduate college roommates participated in this study. All participants were at least 18 years of age and literate. Participants had the option of receiving extra credit in undergraduate psychology courses at the professor’s discretion.
Apparatus/Measures
To measure loneliness, participants completed the UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3) (Appendix A). The scale consists of statements that describe how people may sometimes feel. Participants were asked to indicate how often they feel the way described by writing a number in the space provided. 1 meaning never, 2 meaning rarely, 3 meaning sometimes and 4 meaning always.
To measure social desirability, participants were given the Impression Management subscale of the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (Appendix B). Participants will be asked to rate each question on a 7-point scale; 7 being very true and 1 being very false.
Participants
were also given two identical lists of positive and negative character traits. The first list they were given was used to
indicate characteristics that they believed their roommate would use to
describe them. The second list was used
for indication of the characteristics they would use to describe their roommate. The characteristics were compared among roommates
to see if negative metaperceptions correlate with high scores on the loneliness
scale and low desirability among participants.
Participants were first asked to fill out a consent form. Next participants chose the characteristics that they believe their roommate would use to describe them. They were then given the UCLA Loneliness Scale followed by the second list of characteristics used to describe their roommate. They then filled out the Social Desirability inventory followed by a debriefing, completing the subject’s participation in the study.
Results
To score the results of the UCLA Loneliness Scale the numbers (1-4) are reversed for indicated questions –see or example; for a score of 4, 1 point will be given. For a score of 2, 3 points will be given. The points are then be added up to determine how lonely the participant is.
The social desirability inventory is scored as follows: every odd numbered question that receives a score of 1 or 2 will be given one point and every even numbered question that receives a score of 6 or 7 will receive one point. The points are then added up to determine the participants’ level of social desirability.
The characteristics that participants believe their roommate will use to describe them and the characteristics that are actually chosen were compared and used to decide whether or not the participants have positive or negative metaperceptions. This was then compared to the inventory scores to see if participants who score high on the loneliness scale and low on the social desirability inventory also have negative metaperceptions and vice versa.
The results showed a main effect of loneliness with the scores on the UCLA loneliness test proving to be statistically significant, F (1, 36) = 433.87, p = < .000. Those scoring high on the loneliness scale also tended to score lower on the social desirability scale although there was not a main effect for social desirability. Participants that were shown to have negative metaperceptions scored higher on the loneliness scale. There was not a significant main effect for social desirability, F (1, 36) = ns. There was however an interaction between social desirability and loneliness, F (1, 36) = 14.08, p = < .001. This means that negative metaperceptions depend on if a subject scores high or low on the loneliness scale and whether they score high or low on the social desirability inventory.
Discussion
This study
will contribute to the research done on the effects of loneliness, social
desirability and metaperceptions on each other and separately. Further research could be done concerning
loneliness and social desirability, loneliness and metaperceptions and social
desirability and metaperceptions because it was difficult to find research to
support the hypothesis stating there will be a negative relationship between
loneliness and social desirability with an increase in negative metaperceptions. A main effect was found for loneliness,
whereas participants with negative metaperceptions tended to score high in
loneliness, concurring with the second hypothesis. A main effect was not present for social desirability;
therefore social desirability does not seem to play a major role in whether a
person will have positive or negative metaperceptions. This experiment was not successful in supporting
the hypothesis that participants scoring lower in social desirability are more
likely to have negative metaperceptions.
The main hypothesis stating that participants scoring high on loneliness
and low on social desirability would have an increase in negative
metaperceptions was supported but it is inconclusive whether or not social
desirability played a major role.
This study
was done with a small sample size, so it is a starting point, but to come to
any extremely conclusive results, a larger sample is necessary. This study was also only done between
roommates. There is no way of proving if
the participants have similar metaperceptions with people other than their
roommates. The length of time that
participants have known each other or how long they have been roommates may
also be a factor that was not investigated or accounted for. Another interesting variable to look at is
gender. It is possible that there may be
a difference among genders and there may be a difference in the metaperceptions
one has when encountered by the same or opposite sex. This study contributes to the research done
on social desirability, loneliness and metaperceptions as well as providing a
stepping stone for further research with these variables.
metaperception. Journal
of Personality & Social Psychology, 81 (5), 910-921.
Kalliopuska, M. (1992). Social Desirability Related to Children’s Age, Sex, and
Willingness to Help. Psychological Reports, 70 (2), 479-482.
Kisler, V.A. (1998). Perceptions and metaperceptions of same-sex social interactions in
college women with troubled
eating patterns. Dissertation Abstracts
International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering, 58 (9B), 5124.
Leak, G.K. & Parsons, C.J. (2001). The susceptibility of Three Attachment Style
Measures to Socially Desirable Responding. Social Behavior and Personality, 29 (1), 21-30.
Porrata, J.L. (1993). Differences in Social Desirability Scores of Students in Private and
Public Schools. Psychological Reports, 72 (3 Pt 1), 967-970.
Powling, M., & Hopes, W. (1988). Loneliness, self-characterization and acquaintance in
student groups. Australian Psychologist, 23 (1), 45-53.
Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 12 (1), 121-130.
Table 1.
Level of
Loneliness and Social Desirability for participants found to have Negative
Metaperceptions
|
High Loneliness |
Low Loneliness |
High Social
Desirability |
66.4 |
24.3 |
Low Social
Desirability |
77.1 |
16.5 |
Figure 1. Level of Social Desirability and Loneliness
for Participants with Negative
Metaperceptions.
NEVER-1 RARELY-2 SOMETIMES-3 ALWAYS-4
*1. How often do you feel that you are “in tune” with the people around you?
2.
How often do you feel that you lack companionship?
3.
How often do you feel that there is no one you can
turn to?
4.
How often do you feel alone?
*5.
How often do you feel part of a group of friends?
*6.
How often do you feel that you have a lot in common with the people
around you?
7.
How often do you feel that you are no longer close to
anyone?
8.
How often do you feel that your interests and ideas
are not shared by those around you?
*9.
How often do you feel outgoing and friendly?
*10. How often do you feel close to people?
11.
How often do you feel left out?
12.
How often do you feel that your relationships with
others are not meaningful?
13.
How often do you feel that no one really knows you
well?
14.
How often do you feel isolated from others?
*15. How often do you feel you can find
companionship when you want it?
*16. How often do you feel that there are people
who really understand you?
17.
How often do you feel shy?
18.
How often do you feel that people are around you but
not with you?
*19. How often do you feel that there are people
you can talk to?
*20. How often do you feel that there are people
you can turn to?
Indicate the extent to which you agree with each of the following statements. Use a 7-point scale to indicate your response, with 1 = Not True and 7 = very True.
1. I sometimes
tell lies if I have to.
2. I never
cover up my mistakes.
3. There have
been occasions when I have taken advantage of someone.
4. I never
swear.
5. I sometimes
try to get even rather than forgive and forget.
6. I always obey
laws, even if I’m unlikely to get caught.
7. I have said
something bad about a friend behind his or her back.
8. When I hear
people talking privately, I avoid listening.
9. I have
received too much change from a salesperson without telling him or her.
10. I always
declare everything at customs.
11. When I was
young I sometimes stole things.
12. I have
never dropped litter on the street.
13. I sometimes
drive faster than the speed limit.
14. I never
read sexy books or magazines.
15. I have done
things that I don’t tell other people about.
16. I never
take things that don’t belong to me.
17. I have
taken sick-leave from work or school even though I wasn’t really sick.
18. I have
never damaged a library book or store merchandise without reporting it.
19. I have some
pretty awful habits.
20. I don’t
gossip about other people’s business.